彩神网

 >  > 

2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案

2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案

2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案

次下載官方   年滿12周歲版本:  大小:MB開發商:杭州網易雷火科技有限公司
需優先下載
2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案安裝
普通下載 高速下載
需優先下載
2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案安裝
需跳轉至第三方下載 高速下載
2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案截圖2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案截圖2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案截圖2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案截圖2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案截圖
 小編點評
2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案,現在下載,新用戶還送新人禮包,

2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案2023更新內容

8月1號進行了八月初的年月第一場雅思的考試,相信大家對真題以及答案會非常的日雅感興趣、今天就由出國留學網的思閱試小編為大家介紹2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案。

一、讀考考題解析

2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案

P1 土地沙漠化

2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案

P2 澳大利亞的題答鸚鵡

2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案

P3 多重任務

二、名師點評

1.8月份首場考試的年月難度總體中等,有出現比較多的日雅配對題,沒有出現Heading題,思閱試其余主要以常規的讀考填空,判斷和選擇題為主。題答文章的年月話題和題型搭配也是在劍橋真題中都有跡可循,所以備考重心依然還是日雅劍橋官方真題。

2. 整體分析:涉及環境類(P1)、思閱試動物類(P2)、讀考社科類(P3)。題答

本次考試的P2和P3均為舊題。P2是動物類的話題,題型組合為:段落細節配對+單選+summary填空,難度中等。題型上也延續19年的出題特點,出現配對題,考察定位速度和準確度。P3也出現了段落細節配對,主要是段落細節配對+單選+判斷。三種題型難度中等,但是文章理解起來略有難度。

3. 部分答案及參考文章:

Passage 1:土地沙漠化

題型及答案待確認

Passage 2:澳大利亞的鸚鵡

題型:段落細節配對+單選+Summary填空

技巧分析:由于段落細節配對是完全亂序出題,在定位時需要先做后面的單選題及填空題,最大化利用已讀信息來確定答案,盡量避免重復閱讀,以保證充分的做題時間。

文章內容及題目參考:

A 概況,關于一個大的生物種類

B 一些物種消失的原因,題干關鍵詞:an example of one bird species extinct

C 一種鸚鵡不能自己存活,以捕食另一種鳥為生,吃該鳥類的蛋。題干關鍵詞:two species competed at the expense of oneanother

D 吸引鸚鵡的原因以及鸚鵡嘴的特點。題干關鍵詞:analysis of reasons as Australian landscapeattract parrots

E 植物是如何適應鸚鵡。題干關鍵詞:plants attract birds which make the animal adaptto the environment

F 南半球對英語的影響

G 兩種鸚鵡從環境改變中獲益并存活下來。題干關鍵詞:two species of parrots benefit fromm theenvironment change

H 外來物種及本地鸚鵡

I 鳥類棲息地被破壞以及人類采取的措施

J 作者對于鸚鵡問題的態度

單選題:

why parrots in the whole world are lineal descendants of

選項關鍵詞:continent split from Africa

the writer thinks parrots species beak is for

選項關鍵詞:adjust to their suitable diet

which one is not mentioned

選項關鍵詞:should be frequently maintained

填空題:分布在文章的前兩段

one-sixth

16th century

mapmaker

John Gould

Passage 3:多重任務

題型:段落細節配對+單選+判斷

參考答案及文章

28 F

29I

30C

31B

32G

33C

34B

35A

36YES

37YES

38NO

39NOT GIVEN

40NO

Passage3: multitasking

Multitasking Debate—Can you do them at the same time?

Talking on the phone while driving isn't the only situationwhere we're worse at multitasking than we might like to think we are. Newstudies have identified a bottleneck in our brains that some say means we arefundamentally incapable of true multitasking. If experimental findings reflectreal-world performance, people who think they are multitasking are probablyjust underperforming in all-or at best, all but one -of their parallelpursuits. Practice might improve your performance, but you will never be asgood as when focusing on one task at a time.

The problem, according to René Marois, a psychologist atVanderbilt University in Nashville, Tennessee, is that there's a sticking pointin the brain. To demonstrate this, Marois devised an experiment to locate nteers watch a screen and when a particular image appears, a red circle,say, they have to press a key with their index finger. Different colouredcircles require presses from different fingers. Typical response time is about half a second, and thevolunteers quickly reach their peak performance. Then they learn to listen todifferent recordings and respond by making a specific sound. For instance, whenthey hear a bird chirp, they have to say "ba"; an electronic soundshould elicit a "ko", and so on. Again, no problem. A normal personcan do that in about half a second, with almost no effort. The trouble comeswhen Marois shows the volunteers an image, then almost immediately plays them asound. Now they're flummoxed. "If you show an image and play a sound atthe same time, one task is postponed," he says. In fact,if the second taskis introduced within the half-second or so it takes to process and react to thefirst, it will simply be delayed until the first one is done. The largestdual-task delays occur when the two tasks are presented simultaneously; delaysprogressively shorten as the interval between presenting the tasks lengthens(See Diagram).

There are at least three points where we seem to getstuck, says Marois. The first is in simply identifying what we're looking  can take a few tenths of a second, during which time we are not able tosee and recognise a second item. This limitation is known as the"attentional blink": experiments have shown that if you're watchingout for a particular event and a second one shows up unexpectedly any timewithin this crucial window of concentration, it may register in your visualcortex but you will be unable to act upon it. Interestingly, if you don'texpect the first event, you have no trouble responding to the second. Whatexactly causes the attentional blink is still a matter for debate.

A second limitation is in our short-term visual 's estimated that we can keep track of about four items at a time, fewer ifthey are complex. This capacity shortage is thought to explain, in part, our astonishinginability to detect even huge changes in scenes that are otherwise identical,so-called "change blindness". Show people pairs of near-identicalphotos -say, aircraft engines in one picture have disappeared in the other -andthey will fail to spot the differences (if you don't believe it, check out theclips at /~rensink/flicker/download). Here again, though, thereis disagreement about what the essential limiting factor really is. Does itcome down to a dearth of storage capacity, or is it about how much attention aviewer is paying?

A third limitation is that choosing a response to astimulus -braking when you see a child in the road, for instance,or replyingwhen your mother tells you over the phone that she's thinking of leaving yourdad -also takes brainpower. Selecting a response to one of these things willdelay by some tenths of a second your ability to respond to the other. This iscalled the "response selection bottleneck" theory, first proposed in1952.

Last December, Marois and his colleagues published apaper arguing that this bottleneck is in fact created in two different areas ofthe brain: one in the posterior lateral prefrontal cortex and another in thesuperior medial frontal cortex (Neuron, vol 52, p 1109). They found this byscanning people's brains with functional MRI while the subjects struggled tochoose among eight possible responses to each of two closely timed tasks. Theydiscovered that these brain areas are not tied to any particular sense but aregenerally involved in selecting responses, and they seemed to queue theseresponses when presented with multiple tasks concurrently.

Bottleneck? What bottleneck?

But David Meyer, a psychologist at the University ofMichigan, Ann Arbor, doesn't buy the bottleneck idea. He thinks dual-taskinterference is just evidence of a strategy used by the brain to prioritisemultiple activities. Meyer is known as something of an optimist by his  has written papers with titles like "Virtually perfect time-sharing indual-task performance: Uncorking the central cognitive bottleneck"(Psychological Science, vol 12, p101). His experiments have shown that withenough practice -at least 2000 tries -some people can execute two taskssimultaneously as competently as if they were doing them one after the  suggests that there is a central cognitive processor that coordinates allthis and, what's more, he thinks it uses discretion: sometimes it chooses todelay one task while completing another.

Even with practice, not all people manage to achieve thisharmonious time-share, however. Meyer argues that individual differences comedown to variations in the character of the processor -some brains are just more"cautious", some more "daring". And despite urban legend,there are no noticeable

differences between men and women. So, according to him,it's not a central bottleneck that causes dual-task interference, but rather"adaptive executive control", which "schedules task processesappropriately to obey instructions about their relative priorities and serialorder".

Marois agrees that practice can sometimes eraseinterference effects. He has found that with just 1 hour of practice each dayfor two weeks, volunteers show a huge improvement at managing both his tasks atonce. Where he disagrees with Meyer is in what the brain is doing to achievethis. Marois speculates that practice might give us the chance to find lesscongested circuits to execute a task -rather like finding trusty back streetsto avoid heavy traffic on main roads -effectively making our response to thetask subconscious. After all, there are plenty of examples of subconsciousmultitasking that most of us routinely manage: walking and talking, eating andreading, watching TV and folding the laundry.

But while some dual tasks benefit from practice, otherssimply do not. "Certain kinds of tasks are really hard to do two atonce," says Pierre Jolicoeur at the University of Montreal, Canada, whoalso studies multitasking. Dual tasks involving a visual stimulus andskeletal-motor response (which he dubs "in the eye and out the hand")and an auditory stimulus with a verbal response ("in the ear and out themouth") do seem to be amenable to practice, he says. Jolicoeur has foundthat with enough training such tasks can be performed as well together asapart. He speculates that the brain connections that they use may be somehowspecial, because we learn to speak by hearing and learn to move by looking. Butpair visual input with a verbal response, or sound to motor, and there's nodramatic improvement. "It looks like no amount of practice will allow youto combine these," he says.

For research purposes, these experiments have to be keptsimple. Real-world multitasking poses much greater challenges. Even the upbeatMeyer is sceptical about how a lot of us live our lives. Instant-messaging andtrying to do your homework? "It can't be done," he says. Conducting ajob interview while answering emails? "There's no way you wind up being asgood." Needless to say, there appear to be no researchers in the area ofmultitasking who believe that you can safely drive a car and carry on a phoneconversation. In fact, last year David Strayer at the University of Utah inSalt Lake City reported that people using cellphones drive no better thandrunks (Human Factors, vol 48, p 381). In another study, Strayer found thatusing a hands-free kit did not improve a driver's response time. He concludedthat what distracts a driver so badly is the very act of talking to someone whoisn't present in the car and therefore is unaware of the hazards facing thedriver.

“No researchers believe it's safe to drive a car andcarry on a phone conversation”

It probably comes as no surprise that, generallyspeaking, we get worse at multitasking as we age. According to Art Kramer atthe University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, who studies how ageing affectsour cognitive abilities, we peak in our 20s. Though the decline is slow throughour 30s and on into our 50s, it is there; and after 55, it becomes moreprecipitous. In one study, he and his colleagues had both young and oldparticipants do a simulated driving task while carrying on a conversation. Hefound that while young drivers tended to miss background changes, older driversfailed to notice things that were highly relevant. Likewise, older subjects hadmore trouble paying attention to the more important parts of a scene than youngdrivers.

It's not all bad news for over-55s, though. Kramer alsofound that older people can benefit from practice. Not only did they learn toperform better, brain scans showed that underlying that improvement was achange in the way their brains become active.

Whileit's clear that practice can often make a difference, especially as we age, thebasic facts remain sobering. "We have this impression of an almightycomplex brain," says Marois, "and yet we have very humbling andcrippling limits." For most of our history, we probably never needed to domore than one thing at a time, he says, and so we haven't evolved to be ableto. Perhaps we will in future, though. We might yet look back one day on peoplelike Debbie and Alun as ancestors of a new breed of true multitaskers.

推薦閱讀:

更多  

2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案現在下載,新用戶還送新人禮包

  • [2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案]周年慶狂歡普發禮包3

    截止時間:2024-01-19 22:39:48

    禮包內容:潮流纖維*50,金幣*1

  • [2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案]周年慶狂歡普發禮包1

    截止時間:2024-01-19 22:39:48

    禮包內容:瓶蓋*1000

網友評論

2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案熱門文章

查看全部熱門文章

2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案 歷史版本

熱門「休閑益智」應用推薦

微軟承認部分 WinRE 分區無法承載 1 月更新,導致 Win10、Win11 安裝失敗

344MB

查看
  • 復古賽車最新版下載

    3515MB

    查看
  • 超能英語

    45MB

    查看
  • 西游記釋厄傳

    48889MB

    查看
  • 你可能喜歡更多

    1. 我國首屆“國家工程師獎”重磅出爐:81 名個人、50 個團隊入選  

      3MB

      查看
    2. 導航語音包

      7375MB

      查看
    3. 超能英語

      92MB

      查看
    4. 糖糖網

      47MB

      查看
    點此查看更多好玩游戲排行榜

    2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案相關信息

    時間
    分類
    娛樂 休閑 競技
    標簽
    要求
    Android 5.5.6 以上

    下載了2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案的朋友還下載了

  • 下載2020年8月1日雅思閱讀考試真題答案客戶端 (領禮包看攻略)
    彩88_彩88「官网彩神网」 江苏五分-手机版下载 胜游亚洲|网址 中心购彩-通用app 彩名堂-ios 5分排列3-通用app 彩天下|Welcome